Will drug use rise? Exploring a key concern about decriminalising or regulating drugs

in #drug8 years ago


Introduction
The drug policy reform debate has implications for a
wide range of issues – from crime and health, to the
economy and even the environment. But for many,
the overriding concern is the effect that any policy
change would have on levels of drug consumption (and,
implicitly, the negative consequences that stem from it).
Indeed, the fear that drug use will increase following
any move away from a punitive approach is the most
frequently raised and politically potent of all the
objections to reform, appealing to worries about things
such as the health and welfare of loved ones, increased
public disorder and drugged driving.
Support for maintaining such an approach is typically
predicated on three assumptions:
• That criminalising (or otherwise punishing) drug
users is necessary in order to deter people from
using drugs;
• that enforcement against the supply of drugs
restricts their availability (and, in turn, their use) to
a sufficiently worthwhile extent; and
• that levels of use are a good proxy measure for
levels of harm, both to drug users themselves and
wider society.
This report argues that this rationale for continuing
with an enforcement-led approach to drugs is poorly
supported by empirical research, and that alternative
policies – in particular the decriminalisation of personal
drug possession or the introduction of legally regulated
drug markets – can produce better outcomes while also
avoiding dramatic increases in use. It also makes the
case that overall levels of drug use are not an accurate
indicator of levels of drug-related harm, and should not
be considered as such.
Given the pace at which real-world alternatives to
criminalisation and prohibition have taken hold in
recent years, and the extent to which drug policy
reform has become a mainstream issue, there is now
no shortage of claims and counterclaims about how
different policies affect levels of drug consumption.
This report attempts to cut through this debate,
addressing the widely held concern about increased use
by reviewing the evidence acquired since the modern
international drug control framework was established
in the 1960s, and looking at what is known about
other approaches to managing a range of substances.
Ultimately, the intention is to provide a representative
overview of what is known about the relationship
between drug policy, drug use, and related harms.
But there is also a need to be frank about what is
currently uncertain: the more far-reaching the
departure from the status quo, the more scope there is
for unpredictability. So while there is now a significant
body of research into drug use under prohibition, there
is still no perfect counterfactual to it. Although there
is direct evidence of how decriminalising drugs affects
levels of consumption, there is a much more limited
empirical basis for inferring what the consequences
would be of full-scale legalisation (which in any case
can take many forms). That aside, taken together, the
evidence presented in this report suggests the following
broad conclusions:
• The importance of prevalence of use as an indicator
of the success of drug policy is often overstated,
at the expense of equally or more important
indicators, such as problematic use or drug-related
deaths;
• levels of drug use can be a poor proxy measure
for levels of drug-related harm, and since such
harm is the more important policy priority, more
accurate, alternative indicators should be employed
to measure it;
• the decriminalisation of drug possession for
personal use has, at most, only a marginal impact
on levels of drug use;
• legal drug markets that are highly commercialised
and loosely regulated are likely to lead to significant
increases in drug use compared with levels of use
under prohibition with illicit markets;
• it is possible to create legal drug markets that
are sufficiently regulated and taxed so as to
avoid dramatic increases in drug use (including
problematic use) compared with levels of use under
6
prohibition with illicit markets; and
• the effect of legalisation and regulation (however it
is designed and implemented) on levels of drug use
is likely to vary significantly depending on which
drug or drugs are made legally available.

  1. Drug use: the issues
  2. a) Drug use and harm
    The academic debate on drug policy reform is most
    commonly framed in utilitarian terms (although
    politicians and the public often view it through a
    moral lens). So when participants in this debate make
    their case for a particular approach, they tend not to
    appeal to the intrinsic rightness or wrongness of drugtaking,
    but to the magnitude of positive and negative
    consequences of drug-taking. Of course, different people
    with different values weight these various outcomes
    differently. So questions such as “Would legalisation be
    better than prohibition?” cannot, in the strictest sense,
    be answered scientifically. Nevertheless, the academic
    framing of the debate can and should be informed by
    empirical evidence.
    Taking an archetypal consequentialist approach,
    MacCoun and Reuter (2001; 2011) recommend that
    different drug policy regimes be evaluated by their
    effect on the overall level of harm caused by drug use.
    They identify three factors that determine most of the
    harms caused by drug use: the number of users, the
    average number of doses per user, and the average
    harm produced per incident of drug use. Expressed
    more clearly, the relationship is:
    Total Harm = Prevalence x Intensity x Harmfulness
    Those who support legalising drugs (or other reforms
    that reduce or eliminate penalties for possession)
    typically ascribe most importance to the last element
    of this equation, and include not just harms to the drug
    user, but costs to wider society as well in their definition
    of harmfulness.
    So supporters of reform highlight how prohibition
    and criminalisation can dramatically increase the
    risks associated with drug use itself, as the primary
    risks of many illicit drugs are a product not of their
    pharmacology, but of their being produced and
    supplied by an unregulated criminal market, with users
    directed to the criminal justice system, rather than the
    healthcare system. Street heroin mixed with potent
    adulterants such as fentanyl and used with shared
    needles in unhygienic environments, for example,
    carries far greater risks than pure, pharmaceutical
    heroin (diamorphine) used in a supervised clinical
    setting.
    Added to this, the broader social costs stemming from
    drug use that are created or exacerbated by prohibition
    can include, among other things, the stigma and limited
    life chances that stem from a criminal conviction for
    drug possession, racial disparities in the enforcement
    of punitive drug laws, and the violence and conflict
    generated by street dealers and drug trafficking
    organisations. Such harms, it is argued, could be
    significantly reduced if the drug trade was moved above
    ground and legally regulated, or if users did not run the
    risk of being punished.
    The logical extension of such a focus on harmfulness is
    that if an incident of drug use does not cause significant
    harm to the user or others (or lead to harmful use later),
    then it should not be a concern of public policy. So, it is
    argued, policy should seek to reduce overall harms –
    whether to the user or wider society – from problematic
    use, not necessarily to eliminate use per se. (This is
    discussed in more detail in section 2.c.)
    By contrast, supporters of prohibition focus mostly on
    the first, and to a lesser extent the second, component
    of the equation above, rejecting changes to the status
    quo out of concern that drug use would increase if
    sanctions were reduced and/or a legal supply of drugs
    was established. After all, decriminalisation or legal
    regulation might cause total harm to rise if, despite
    reducing the average harm per incident of use, this
    was outweighed by a sufficiently large increase in
    the number of such incidents. And depending on the
    7
    extent of such an increase in use, that could ultimately
    mean higher levels of drug dependence, greater
    numbers of intoxication-related accidents, and/or
    drug-related deaths.
    Both sides of the drug policy debate are therefore
    sometimes guilty of neglecting key factors that
    contribute to the overall amount of drug-related harm.
    Reformers are often reluctant to engage with the
    issue of whether, under a less punitive approach, use
    could rise to what would be, on net, more damaging
    levels; and advocates of prohibition rarely consider the
    proposition that an increase in use could be justified by
    a sufficiently large reduction in average harm.
    As alluded to above, use reduction has tended to be the
    dominant aim of drug policy, with the priority usually
    a reduction in the number of people who use drugs
    (rather than a reduction in the intensity of their use).
    In the US, for example, the national strategy of the
    Office of National Drug Control Policy (2007: 1) has
    historically been framed almost exclusively in terms of
    reducing the prevalence of use (although this has begun
    to change in recent years).
  3. b) The dominance of prevalence
    The prevalence of drug use (the number of people
    using drugs in a given population) is clearly a legitimate
    concern under any policy model, but its status as the
    key indicator of the success or failure of drug policy
    stems in part from a simple lack of more accurate drug
    data systems. The production and use of illegal drugs is
    not something that market participants happily report
    to the authorities. Hence there is nothing like the kind
    of administrative data that exists for tracking activity in
    other, legal markets.
    Drug arrests and drug-related deaths were originally
    used to monitor trends in drug supply and use, but
    these measures are highly imprecise: variations in the
    number of drug arrests, rather than signifying changes
    in consumption or production, can simply indicate
    changes in enforcement practices; and drug-related
    deaths are often multiply determined – an intoxicated
    driver killing a pedestrian may be recorded as a traffic
    death, rather than as a consequence of drug use.
    So when drug use grew to the point of being a
    central concern in the 1960s and 1970s, there was a
    concerted effort to devise better data systems. This
    led to the development of school-based surveys of
    drug use by youth, and surveys of drug use in the
    general population (often called “household population
    surveys”).
    Data limitations therefore played a major role in the
    prevalence of drug use assuming such importance
    in the evaluation of drug policy. But this situation is
    also partly a function of a culture that has, whether
    explicitly or implicitly, defined the consumption of
    certain drugs as intrinsically wrong and damaging.
    Public perceptions of illicit drug use (and those who
    engage in it) have been distorted by more than a
    century of moral panics, early examples of which were
    driven by racial prejudice and, in the United States in
    particular, by an influential Temperance movement
    that considered the use of all psychoactive drugs –
    including alcohol – as a root cause of social decay
    (Berridge and Edwards, 1981; Jay, 2002; Musto, 1999;
    Courtwright, 2005).
    But while attitudes towards alcohol use changed
    following America’s failed experiment with alcohol
    prohibition, the use of other, more “foreign” drugs
    has remained highly stigmatised. Prohibition-era
    rhetoric was (and often still is) simply applied to a
    different range of substances, as can be seen in the UN
    convention that underpins today’s international drug
    control regime. It describes drug addiction as a “serious
    evil … fraught with social and economic danger to
    mankind”, one that the international community has
    a “duty to … combat” (United Nations, 1961: 1). This set
    the tone for the “war on drugs” declared by President
    Richard Nixon in the 1970s.
    Against this backdrop, in which the use of certain
    drugs is conceived of as a threat to the very fabric of
    society, it is perhaps unsurprising that drug policy has
    8
    overwhelmingly focused on prevalence reduction.
    “Harm reduction” approaches – which seek to make
    drug use safer, rather than just reduce or eliminate
    it – have faced significant political obstacles, arguably
    because they focus less on the simple fact of whether
    a person uses drugs, and more on whether a person’s
    drug use is having negative consequences (Harm
    Reduction International, 2016).
  4. c) Prevalence of use as a measure of
    total harm
    Clearly, drug use can, in itself, cause substantial harm to
    individuals, but it is by no means an inevitability. There
    are different types of drugs and drug-using behaviours,
    motivated by different priorities, which have different
    outcomes. It is far from the case that everyone who
    takes drugs becomes a chronic, dependent user, whose
    consumption is high-risk and likely to cause themselves
    and/or others harm.
    The prevalence of drug use can be estimated by a
    number of methods, including wastewater-testing,
    or testing of arrestees, but is usually calculated
    from survey data. This is then augmented by
    population-specific research, such as school-based
    surveys (although these are less consistent across
    jurisdictions, complicating national comparisons).
    Many countries have established surveys that focus
    specifically on drug-taking behaviour, while others
    incorporate questions about drug use into general
    health surveys, or, as in the case of England and
    Wales, into national crime surveys. The context of
    the survey inevitably influences the type of questions
    that are asked and who responds to it (EMCDDA,
    2009: 15) .
    The European Model Questionnaire recommends that
    countries ask questions on a minimum of six drugs:
    cannabis, ecstasy, cocaine, heroin, amphetamines
    and LSD. However, some drug use is rarely captured;
    certain surveys now include questions on some
    new psychoactive substances (NPS), but not all (the
    bewildering array of such products making it all but
    impossible), while the use of diverted prescription
    drugs is generally poorly monitored, leaving a messy
    and incomplete picture of drug use trends.
    Moreover, the very nature of these surveys leads to
    underestimates of the true extent of drug use: people
    are generally reluctant to admit to illegal activity,
    and added to this, the most high-risk, problematic
    drug users are unlikely to be represented in surveys
    of households, given that they often live chaotic
    lives (Home Office, 2006). The most commonly used
    types of surveys therefore bias prevalence estimates
    towards conservatism, even if they do so consistently,
    in a way that can reliably reveal trends.
    Three indicators are typically used to measure
    the prevalence of drug use in a given country or
    jurisdiction: lifetime prevalence, last-year prevalence
    and last-month prevalence. Of these three indicators,
    lifetime prevalence – the number of people who
    have ever used drugs – is the least useful, because
    by definition, such use cannot be reversed: even if
    people stop using drugs, they will remain lifetime
    users. Lifetime prevalence therefore tends to rise
    consistently over time. (This measure is, however,
    considered more useful for schoolchildren, as
    initiation into most drug use occurs in teenage years,
    so lifetime prevalence can provide a better snapshot
    of drug-use trends among this group.) Last-year and
    last-month prevalence are better proxy measures of
    current levels of drug use, and should be used to track
    emerging consumption trends (EMCDDA, 2002).
    Measuring the prevalence of drug use
    9
    In fact, the vast majority of people who use drugs would
    not fit this description. The United Nations Office
    on Drugs and Crime (UNODC, 2015) – the agency
    that oversees the international prohibitionist drug
    control system – estimates that approximately 90%
    of people who use illicit drugs worldwide do so nonproblematically.1
    Hence prevalence of use alone is not
    a particularly useful or accurate indicator of harm: it
    conflates both problematic and non-problematic drug
    consumption, even though the risks associated with
    each, and the responses that they call for, differ greatly.
    A 17-year-old heavy user of crystal methamphetamine
    who is supplied by a criminal market and commits
    crimes to feed their habit, risks causing far greater
    harm to themselves and others than does a middleaged,
    occasional cannabis user who grows their own
    plants. If the latter ceased their drug-taking while
    the former did not, the prevalence of drug use in this
    sample would fall by half, and yet would produce little,
    if any, change in total levels of health and social harm.
    While this distinction between the relative harms
    associated with different types of drug use is
    acknowledged in the academic debate on reform, it is
    frequently overlooked in the public debate – politicians’
    press releases and media soundbites talk all too often
    about the prevalence of drug use as it were a single,
    homogenous phenomenon.
    That overall prevalence of use is a poor proxy measure
    for aggregate harm can be seen from the situation in
    the UK. In 2014, Prime Minister David Cameron said:
    “We have a policy which actually is working in Britain:
    drug use is coming down” (quoted in Wallis Simons,
    2014). The second part of this claim was essentially
    accurate at that time: the long-term picture was of a
    decline in the overall prevalence of drug use in the
    UK (although, since around 2010, use had actually
  5. The UNODC defines “problem drug users” as “people who engage
    in the high-risk consumption of drugs, for example people
    who inject drugs, people who use drugs on a daily basis and/or
    people diagnosed with drug use disorders or as drug-dependent
    …” http://www.unodc.org/documents/wdr2014/World_Drug_
    Report_2014_web.pdf xvii
    stabilised [Home Office, 2015]). Yet the picture is more
    complicated – and less impressive – when the broader
    context is examined.
    The decline in overall levels of drug use that occurred
    in the previous decade was driven mostly by a
    reduction in the number of young people (aged 16-24)
    using cannabis, the most widely used illicit drug (see
    box, p. 10, for speculation on the causes of this trend).
    The use of other drugs has, however, proven more
    resilient. Between 2012/13 and 2014/15, the number of
    young people who took ecstasy in the past year almost
    doubled,to levels not seen since 2003, and young
    people’s cocaine use also sharply increased over the
    same period (Home Office, 2015).
    But despite this, overall prevalence was either in
    decline or stable. So does this consumption trend
    indicate that UK drug policy is “working”? A more
    useful indicator suggests not. Fewer people using drugs
    does not mean fewer people being harmed by drugs.
    The drug-induced mortality rate among adults in the
    UK was 55.9 deaths per million in 2013, almost three
    times the most recent European average of 19.2 deaths
    per million (EMCDDA, 2016). Official 2014 estimates
    for the whole of the UK are not yet available, but in
    England and Wales, drug-induced deaths reached the
    highest levels ever recorded (ONS, 2016). This trend is
    being driven by a complex mixture of factors, mostly
    related to opioid consumption. Nationally and locally,
    an ageing population of users, changes in available
    treatment options, and batches of particularly strong or
    contaminated opioids are behind the rise. These deaths,
    and the reasons for them, underline the importance
    of not taking headline figures on use as a simple
    determinant of policy success or failure.
    It is also notable that, historically, governments tend
    to cite any change in levels of drug use as evidence to
    support their position: when use falls, it is heralded
    as a triumph that renders any debate about reform
    irrelevant; but when use rises, calls are made for
    enforcement efforts to be intensified. As such, a fixation
    on levels of use can often shut down vital discussions
    about policies that could deliver better outcomes.
    10
    From 2000-14, levels of cannabis use among 15- to
    34-year-olds remained stable or increased in many
    European countries, including France, Sweden,
    Denmark and Finland (EMCDDA, 2015b). In England
    and Wales, however, consumption among this group
    fell by almost half over the same period (EMCDDA,
    2015a). Listed below are several possible explanations
    that have been put forward for this trend. (It should
    be noted that these are all speculative explanations,
    reflected by the references given for them, most of
    which are to news articles that feature interviews
    with drug policy experts, rather than peer-reviewed
    research. These proposed explanations should
    therefore be treated with caution, although they
    usefully illustrate how drug use is influenced by
    myriad factors, rather than policy alone.)
    • The decline in tobacco smoking in the UK. The
    fall in the number of tobacco smokers, which
    predates the fall in cannabis use, may have
    helped de-normalise smoking more generally.
    And since Britons commonly consume cannabis
    in a mix with tobacco, fewer non-smokers may
    be taking up the drug (The Economist, 2015).
    • The rise of high-strength “skunk”. The UK
    cannabis market is increasingly dominated
    by more potent varieties of the drug that are
    unappealing to novice users given the higher
    probability of negative experiences (McVeigh and
    O’Neill, 2012).
    • A cultural shift. The decline in young people’s
    cannabis use is mirrored by similar, but less
    pronounced, declines in alcohol consumption and
    the use of other drugs, pointing to the possible
    emergence of a more abstemious and risk-averse
    youth culture (McVeigh and O’Neill, 2012;
    Benedictus, 2011).
    • The rise of the internet and social media. Smart
    phones, the internet and gaming consoles may
    be having an effect on young people’s behaviour
    in relation to drugs (Goldhill, 2014; Barnes, 2012).
    The rise of such technology may be reducing
    boredom or the amount of “dead time” that might
    otherwise be filled by casual drug use, or it may
    be reducing real-world interactions in which
    drug sharing or peer pressure come into play
    (Cabinet Office and Department of Health, 2015).
    Online photo sharing may also be increasing
    image and body consciousness, turning people
    off more unhealthy or unattractive drug-using
    behaviours.
    • The rise of new psychoactive substances. Some
    young people may be switching from real
    cannabis to synthetic cannabinoids, such as
    “Spice”, which mimic its effects and were, until
    a blanket ban in 2016, relatively cheaply and
    legally available via high-street “head shops”.
    Comparatively little is known about levels of use
    of such substances (EMCDDA, 2015b).
    • Societal changes. More young people are living
    at their parents’ home than in previous years
    (ONS, 2011), which may be limiting opportunities
    for illicit behaviour such as cannabis use. The
    employment rate for 16-17-year-olds has also
    declined substantially over the past decade (ONS,
    2013), potentially meaning fewer teenagers have
    the money to purchase cannabis.
    Declining cannabis use in England and Wales: possible causes
    11
  6. d) Beneficial drug use?
    The use of illicit drugs, even when moderate, controlled,
    and recognised as being relatively low-risk, is still
    typically characterised as antisocial or having net
    adverse effects. There is a general reluctance to accept
    or admit that currently illicit substances may confer
    any benefits to users or wider society.
    In contrast, this point is often acknowledged with
    regard to the use of licit drugs. The UK government’s
    2012 Alcohol Strategy, for example, says:
    “In moderation, alcohol consumption can have a
    positive impact on adults’ wellbeing, especially where
    this encourages sociability. Well-run community
    pubs and other businesses form a key part of the
    fabric of neighbourhoods, providing employment
    and social venues in our local communities. And
    a profitable alcohol industry enhances the UK
    economy” (HM Government, 2012: 3).
    It is difficult to imagine any government making a
    similar statement in reference to illicit drugs, but the
    logic is sound: most currently illegal substances can
    be – and most often are – consumed responsibly, with
    negligible harm resulting either to users or wider
    society. (And where harm to wider society does occur,
    it is often a product of the criminal nature of the drug
    trade, rather than drug use itself.)
    All of this is not to say that policy should not attempt
    to deter people (particularly young people or other
    vulnerable populations) from using drugs – especially in
    high-risk ways; it is simply to highlight the distinction
    between drug use and drug harm, and place this
    distinction within the context of the reasons why
    people take drugs in the first place.
    Pleasure is the “great unmentionable”(Hunt and Evans,
  1. in drug policy research and the public debate
    on reform (Moore, 2008; Holt and Treloar, 2008) but
    given the central role it plays in motivating various
    forms of drug use, it must be factored into thinking
    around policy responses to changing levels of use. That
    pleasure – or any quasi-medical or lifestyle “benefit”,
    such as relaxation, stress relief, or enhanced social
    experiences – is the primary desired outcome of drug
    use is indisputable, and for most drug users suggests
    a willingness to bear at least some degree of risk to
    achieve that outcome (Ritter, 2014).
    Casual / non-problematic use
    Recreational, casual or other use that has
    negligible harmful health or social effects, e.g.,
    moderate cannabis, cocaine or MDMA use in
    social settings
    Chronic dependence
    Use that has become habitual and compulsive
    despite negative health and social effects, e.g.,
    long-term opioid dependence, which is funded
    through acquisitive crime
    Beneficial use
    Use that has positive health, spiritual or
    social effects, e.g., medical pharmaceuticals,
    stimulants – such as coffee or tea – to increase
    alertness, sacramental use of ayahuasca,
    therapeutic use of MDMA
    Problematic use
    Use that begins to have negative
    consequences for individual, friends / family,
    or wider society, e.g., use leading to impaired
    driving, binge consumption, harmful
    methods of administration
    Spectrum of psychoactive substance use
    Adapted from: British Columbia Ministry of Health Services (2004)
    12
    In fact, there are countless pleasure-seeking activities
    – be it sport, sex, sunbathing, or the consumption of
    sugar or fatty foods – that people are willing to engage
    in despite their risks. Society therefore accepts that a
    certain level of risk is permissible in order to achieve
    certain pleasures. Sometimes – in the case of, say,
    motorbike racing or some extreme sports – the risks are
    very high, and may not only be tolerated, but viewed as
    an intrinsic part of the experience.
    But with drugs, the overriding concern with reducing
    use has marginalised a more pragmatic and constructive
    debate about what motivates consumption, what
    level of drug-related risk or harm should be tolerated,
    and which policy approaches can help manage and
    moderate those risks. Instead, unlike with other risky
    pleasure-seeking activities, there is a range of groups
    and institutions dedicated to exaggerating potential
    harms and denying benefits. Rugby or American
    football, as played by the typical school student, is likely
    to carry far more risk of damaging the developing
    brain than cannabis, but there is no agency devoted to
    publicising those risks in the media.
  1. The impact of different
    policy models on the
    prevalence of drug use
    There are various approaches that can be taken in
    response to the use of currently illicit drugs, many of
    which – such as the introduction of harm reduction
    services or the intensification of enforcement efforts –
    can take place under a range of policy models. Rather
    than consider the impact of these kinds of relatively
    more incremental reforms on the prevalence of drug
    use, this report focuses on how, if at all, drug use differs
    under the three most commonly discussed overarching
    policy regimes. It will first look at drug use under
    prohibition, then under two options for reform – the
    decriminalisation of personal drug possession and the
    legal regulation of drug markets. It should be noted
    that there can be significant variation in the design and
    implementation of each of these policy models (drug
    prohibition in China is far more punitive than in the
    UK, for example). There is not space here to explore
    all the possible forms these policies can take, so this
    As discussed, care must be taken when inferring
    levels of drug-related harm from levels of drug use.
    The two are far from equivalent; that is why other
    indicators must be considered in order to determine
    whether drug policy is promoting public health and
    safety effectively.
    Evidence from studies of alcohol use illustrate this
    point further. As one cross-sectional survey of the
    effects of alcohol in Russia, the Czech Republic and
    Poland found: “Overall alcohol consumption does
    not suffice as an estimate of alcohol related problems
    at the population level” (Bobak et al., 2004). This
    was because the Czech Republic, despite having
    significantly higher levels of alcohol use than
    Russia, experienced far less alcohol-related harm, as
    measured by the proportion of people whose drinking
    causes difficulties with, among other things, their
    relationships, work, physical, psychological or mental
    health and financial circumstances.
    Emphasising the way in which culture and patterns
    of consumption are important determinants of harm,
    this result was attributed to the fact that Czechs,
    while drinking more often and more as a whole,
    consume relatively small amounts per occasion,
    whereas Russians’ drinking is more concentrated
    – they drink less frequently, but consume large
    amounts of alcohol when they do (and such patterns
    of binge use tend to be associated with higher health
    and social harms). Studies of other countries have also
    found that low overall levels of alcohol use are by no
    means a guarantee of low levels of alcohol-related
    harm (Rossow, 2001; Ramstedt, 2001; Christie, 1965;
    Poikolainen, 1977; Room, 1974; Norström, 2001).

Coin Marketplace

STEEM 0.19
TRX 0.18
JST 0.031
BTC 89200.83
ETH 3215.57
USDT 1.00
SBD 2.84